Skip to main content
  • Original research
  • Open access
  • Published:

Multivariate analysis of brain metabolism reveals chemotherapy effects on prefrontal cerebellar system when related to dorsal attention network

Abstract

Background

Functional brain changes induced by chemotherapy are still not well characterized. We used a novel approach with a multivariate technique to analyze brain resting state [18 F]FDG-PET in patients with lymphoma, to explore differences on cerebral metabolic glucose rate between chemotherapy-treated and non-treated patients.

Methods

PET/CT scan was performed on 28 patients, with 14 treated with systemic chemotherapy. We used a support vector machine (SVM) classification, extracting the mean metabolism from the metabolic patterns, or networks, that discriminate the two groups. We calculated the correct classifications of the two groups using the mean metabolic values extracted by the networks.

Results

The SVM classification analysis gave clear-cut patterns that discriminate the two groups. The first, hypometabolic network in chemotherapy patients, included mostly prefrontal cortex and cerebellar areas (central executive network, CEN, and salience network, SN); the second, which is equal between groups, included mostly parietal areas and the frontal eye field (dorsal attention network, DAN). The correct classification membership to chemotherapy or not chemotherapy-treated patients, using only one network, was of 50% to 68%; however, when all the networks were used together, it reached 80%.

Conclusions

The evidenced networks were related to attention and executive functions, with CEN and SN more specialized in shifting, inhibition and monitoring, DAN in orienting attention. Only using DAN as a reference point, indicating the global frontal functioning before chemotherapy, we could better classify the subjects. The emerging concept consists in the importance of the investigation of brain intrinsic networks and their relations in chemotherapy cognitive induced changes.

Background

Cognitive changes in cancer patients after adjuvant chemotherapy (CHT) have been reported since the mid 1970s, with systematic research starting in the early 1990s. Most neuropsychological studies on CHT-treated cancer survivors have reported cognitive impairments in multiple domains such as executive functions, learning, memory, attention, verbal fluency and speed of information processing. The CHT effects ranges from small to moderate, involving mostly the cognitive functions subserved by frontal lobes [13].

PET and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) brain studies have provided evidences that CHT can induce both structural (white matter [48] or grey matter [6, 7, 9] damages) and functional changes (hypometabolism [10] or hypoactivation [1113]) in areas correlated with attention, concentration and/or memory (such as the prefrontal, cingulate and parahippocampal cortex). Longitudinal neuropsychological [14, 15] and neuroimaging [5, 7] studies pointed to a total reversibility after 1 to 3 years, opposed to cross-sectional studies [10, 12, 16, 17] that evidenced, in some patients, a long-lasting damage (5 to 10 years). It is likely that chemotherapy generates a subtle damage, largely reversible for the resolving power of our techniques, but that could constitute, in the long course, a risk factor for the onset of cognitive impairment in some genetically and environmentally vulnerable patients [18, 19].

All the cited neuroimaging studies used univariate approaches to identify the presence of focal or local brain changes induced by chemotherapy, trying to find areas in which the null hypothesis could be discarded. In this study, we investigated the chemotherapy effects starting from the brain total activity, instead of comparing two groups of treated and not treated patients for single voxels. We considered the pattern of intrinsic covariation of different brain regions using them to map representative brain characteristics onto the two groups. It is well known that the resting brain contains the temporal overlapping of many spatially independent networks [20], which activity during behavioural tasks is correlated with brain cognitive and emotional functions [2123]. This is possibly an intrinsic characteristic of the brain architecture, which is a depiction of the need of integration of different areas to substantiate the mind abilities [24].

The cognitive functions that are more fogged by chemotherapy (attention, concentration, memory, speed processing and executive functions) could be suggestive of the networks involved that will be the most representative of these functions [22, 2528] which are the following:

  1. 1

    The central executive network (CEN), left and right parts, consists mainly of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), Brodmann area (BA), 44/45/46, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, BA 8, 9, inferior and superior parietal lobule, BA 7, supramarginal and angular gyrus, BA 39/40 and cerebellum crus 1 and crus 2.

  2. 2

    Salience network (SN) consists mainly of the medial frontal cortex, BA 32, the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, BA 24, anterior prefrontal cortex, BA 10, 11, DLPFC, BA 46, the frontoinsular cortex, BA 47/12, the thalamus and the cerebellum VI and crus 1.

  3. 3

    Dorsal attentional network (DAN) consists mainly of the superior parietal lobule, BA 5, 7, supramarginal gyrus, BA 40 the frontal eye fields (FEF), BA 6 and cerebellum VI.

We employed the multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA), a technique involving searching through data to identify patterns of features that are highly predictive of different conditions. MVPA has been successfully applied in many different medicine fields, for example, in gene selection for large cluster analyses [29] or in detection of ventricular fibrillation [30]. In neuroimaging literature, the use of MVPA for functional MRI analyses (fMRI) [31, 32] has become more and more popular after the work of Haxby et al. [33]. The voxels’ activation patterns in the ventral temporal cortex, during a vision task, were distinctive of the category of objects viewed by the experimental subjects (drawing attention to the so-called mind reading field of study [34]). This could be considered a revival in fMRI of the multivariate techniques introduced in PET a decade before [35, 36]. In our case, data features were the regional cerebral metabolic glucose rate values, obtained from brain resting state [18 F]FDG-PET in lymphoma patients and MVPA extracted spatial patterns, representative of the two different groups brain states (treated and untreated patients).

MVPA, compared to the univariate analysis, could detect subtle distributed effects and might consider the covariance between distinct brain areas as a signature of distributed brain networks. The aim of the study was to uncover the effects of chemotherapy on brain intrinsic networks enclosed in the resting ongoing activity.

Methods

Patients

Cancer patients were enrolled among those who were planned to undergo a whole-body [18 F]FDG-PET/CT on a clinically routine basis for cancer staging or to monitor the disease after treatment.

Patients were considered eligible if they did not have symptoms of neurological and psychiatric disorders and medications that could potentially alter the neuropsychological performances and/or brain metabolism. We also excluded patients who had, in the previous 2 years, pharmacological or psychological treatments that could potentially alter the brain metabolism or who had serious physical effects after the chemotherapy treatment.

Eligible patients gave written informed consent to participate to the project, approved by the ethical committee of AOU San Giovanni Battista University Hospital in Turin.

Among 28 enrolled patients, 14 were previously treated with systemic CHT and 14 patients were not treated (no CHT). The two subgroups were balanced for age (mean 52 years old), gender (12 males, 2 females) and education level. All selected patients were evaluated and treated in the same Haematology Oncology Department. In both groups, majority of the patients had a non-Hodgkin's lymphoma (NHL) (n = 12, 86%), while minority had a Hodgkin’s lymphoma (HL) (n = 2, 14%). Ten patients (36%, 5 had CHT, 5 had no CHT) referred weight loss, febrile episodes or night sweats during the last 6 months; nobody presented fever on the day of the scanning. Fifteen patients (54%, 4 had CHT, 11 had no CHT) had lymphoma localisations outside the brain at PET total body examination. Nobody presented morphological alterations at brain CT examination.

The CHT group underwent to conventional standard-dose chemotherapy (the mean number of cycles was 6 and the mean time elapsed from the treatment was 7 months).

The hydroxydaunorubicin (Adriamycin), bleomycin, vinblastine, dacarbazine (ABVD) chemotherapy was used as a first-line protocol in all the HL patients. The cyclophosphamide, hydroxydaunorubicin and vincristine (Oncovin), prednisone or prednisolone associated with the monoclonal antibody rituximab (R-CHOP) were used as first-line protocol in seven NHL patients (58%).

Second-line treatments were used in five NHL patients (42%), in three cases, in association with rituximab: cyclophosphamide, mitoxantrone (Novantrone), Oncovin, prednisone (CNOP); vincristine, Adriamycin, cyclophosphamide, Oncovin and prednisone (VACOP); and etoposide, Oncovin, cyclophosphamide, hydroxydaunorubicin, (EPOCH). Corticosteroid therapy was used in about 86% (n = 12) and immunotherapy in about 71% of patients (n = 10). All protocol regimens were considered similar for toxicity and side effects by the oncologists who took care of patients.

All patients underwent to a screening battery, in the same morning before the PET acquisition, to exclude cognitive impairment, anxiety and depression containing the following tests: Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE), Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS), Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS), Distress Thermometer (DT).

PET scanning

In a quiet waiting room, the participants, lying in a supine position, were asked to refrain from moving and instructed ‘to keep their eyes closed, to not engage in any structured mental activity such as counting, rehearsing, etc. and to avoid falling asleep’. They were then blindfolded and ear plugged and received intravenously about 4.5 to 5.5 MBq kg−1 of 2-deoxy-2-[18 F]fluoro-d-glucose. About 30 min later, PET/CT scan was performed by a Philips Gemini scanner (Philips Medical System, Cleveland, Ohio, USA). The brain scan acquisition time was 20 min. Coronal, sagittal and transverse data sets were reconstructed using a 3D iterative technique (row action maximum likelihood algorithm, RAMLA-3D) and corrected with single scatter simulation. Reconstructed brain images had a dimension of 128 × 128 × 90 voxels (2 × 2 × 2 mm3).

PET brain images were preprocessed using SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) running on MATLAB 7.5 software (Mathworks, Natick, Massachusetts). All images were nonlinearly and spatially normalized into the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space and smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel with 12 mm full width half maximum. We normalized the count of each voxel to the mean count of a standardized pontine region of interest (ROI). The ROI was a rectangular multislice region (−8 mm < x < 8 mm, −32 mm < y < −24 mm, −44 mm < z < −34 mm, MNI space) sampling 144 voxels on the central pontine region and manually drawn on the PET MNI template using the MRIcron application (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/mricron). The same ROI was employed on each spatial normalized and smoothed brain image to sample the pons mean and then to scale voxel values of each subject individually with the image calculation SPM tool.

SVM classification analysis

Support vector machine (SVM) is a supervised classification machine-learning algorithm [37] commonly used as neuroimaging MVPA [31, 32]. SVM has typically two phases: training and test phase. In the training phase, SVM receives as input a number of cases or instances, described by the values of distinctive features or characteristics, specifying the membership of every case to one of two different groups or categories. For example, we could try to teach to the machine how to classify human gender based on height, hair length and waist to hip ratio features. SVM, in the multidimensional space of the N features, searches the best hyperplane H (N-1 dimensional) separating the classes without error (e.g., females are generally short, long hair and wide hips creatures). Since there could exist more than one solution to this separation problem, SVM searches the only H with the maximum margin, which means that the distance or separation of the two classes are maximized by the choice of H. This algorithm selects from many possible solutions the optimal one determined by the most informative training examples (called the support vectors), those are the nearest to the margin (borderline cases). This formulation is called hard margin SVM and implies that the problem is well posed, separable and that a solution exists. But there are problems in which some instances could be so misleading, falling quite distant from the representative mean of the group (e.g., a masculine shaved basket girl player), that it does not exist a solution. The soft margin SVM (C-SVM or nu-SVM) introduces a regularization parameter that allows some examples to fall inside the margin and to ignore them in the best choice of H. In the proposed example of the shaved girl, that case will be ignored in the choice of H and the margin extended to include her. Generally, the value of the parameter should be searched by trials and errors, using some validation criteria to optimize it. After training phase, the algorithm has selected the optimal hyperplane H separating the classes that can be described in the following equation:

H : w v i + b = 0 ,

where w are the computed learning weights, b is an offset and v i is the classes features.

In the test phase, another sample of cases is classified using the weights and assigning the membership to a class based on the positive or negative sum of the weighted features (this means to which ‘side’ of the hyperplane the example falls). Therefore, we could verify the accuracy and the generality of the computed solution. Features with a weight near zero tend to have little impact in the classification.

We applied the SVM onto the patient's whole brain PET, where every voxel was considered as a feature and the two classes were CHT or no CHT groups. We chose SVM because it is still effective in high-dimensional spaces, where the number of features is greater than the number of samples (we had 28 cases and thousands of features or voxels). More importantly, it can be taken into account the full spatial pattern of brain activity which is measured simultaneously at many locations and exploiting its inherent multivariate nature which is intrinsically organized in networks.

Analyses were performed with an in-house Matlab-based MVPA toolbox, which adopted LIBSVM, an easy-to-use and efficient library for SVM classification and regression [38]. In particular, we used a nu-SVM classification machine and a grid searching of the best parameter of the machine. The training and test data consist of 28 subjects FDG-PET whole brain data. To test the accuracy of the classification we used n-fold validation scheme leaving out two random subjects for the test phase after the training with the remaining 26 subjects, repeating 14 times the procedure. After the best choice of H, we had a large number of weights (one for every brain voxels) so we kept only the voxels for which |w| > > 0, discarding the other as less important for the classification. We split the weights into two patterns or networks: one of high negative weights and one of high positive weights; these corresponded to the voxels that are important for discrimination between the groups. For the description of these networks, we used the viewer xjView8, an SPM extension that could also output the cortical and Brodmann areas contained within an input brain map in the MNI space ( http://www.alivelearn.net/xjview8).

Then we extracted the mean metabolism from the two patterns of weights and we compared, with two independent sample t tests, the values between CHT and no CHT groups. We also compared the mean metabolism with analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) models using potentially confounding as covariate (depression, age and time elapsed from treatment). We calculated the correct classifications of the subjects' membership to CHT or no CHT groups using only the mean metabolic values extracted by the two networks.

We tested the association between clinical, demographic variables and extracted metabolic values with χ2 test, two-sample independent t test, ANCOVA, Pearson's correlations, Kendall's tau-b, using SPSS 13.0; p < 0.05 was considered as significant.

Results

The demographic and clinical characteristics of patients are shown in Table  1. In all patients, except four, HADS scores were under the clinical relevant cut-off of 8 (mildly anxious and depressed and equally distributed among groups). All patients were cognitively spared (MMSE > 26).

Table 1 Clinical and demographic characteristics

There were no significant differences between groups in level of distress, anxiety, depression, demographic or clinical features (p > 0.05).

Patients with and without symptoms, who did not differ in brain metabolism and in clinical and demographic features, were equally distributed among CHT and no CHT groups and had similar presence/absence of PET localizations outside the brain (p > 0.05).

SVM patterns

The nu-SVM classification analysis had a good generalization and could discriminate the two groups with an accuracy of 65% (computed by n-fold cross validation, greater than chance >57%). The two patterns of more weighting voxels, significant for discrimination, are shown in Figure  1, in red the pattern of high positive weights, in blue the high negative.

Figure 1
figure 1

Patterns of SVM classification analysis. Weights of SVM classification analysis between no CHT and CHT groups overlaid on canonical MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) brain template. Images are in neurological convention (left is left). In red the positive weights pattern evidenced a prefrontal cerebellar system (PCS) composed primarily by salience network (SN) and secondarily by central executive network (CEN). In blue the negative weights pattern evidence the dorsal attention network (DAN).

The red pattern was symmetrical and included many prefrontal cortices and cerebellar areas: anterior cingulated cortex (ACC, BA 24, 32), medial prefrontal cortex (BA 8, 9, 10) dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC, BA 45, 46), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC, BA 11, 47), anterior insula (AI, BA 48), putamen, thalamus, vermis VIII-IX, crus 1, crus 2, lobules VIIB, VIII. It included some additional clusters in the middle cingulated cortex, cuneus, precuneus, calcarine scissure, superior temporal and supramarginal gyrus. The largest part of the pattern, with the highest weights, was contained in front of genu of corpus callosum: ACC and medial prefrontal cortex (BA 10, 32) and in the posterior cerebellum (crus 1 and crus 2). From a network point of view, the pattern was largely overlapping with the SN, but included also parts of the left and right CEN and other additional small clusters. We will refer to this pattern together as the prefrontal cerebellar system (PCS).

The blue pattern was symmetrical and included mostly parietal areas: postcentral gyrus (BA 2, 3), paracentral (BA 5), inferior, superior parietal lobules (BA 7), with additional clusters in precentral cortex (BA 4), particularly the frontal eye field (FEF, BA 6), and in brainstem pontine nuclei. This pattern was virtually indistinguishable from the DAN.

Classification and groups differences

CHT patients had significantly less positive PET localizations outside brain compared to no CHT patients (p = 0.01).

The DAN mean metabolism (Figure  2 right) was very similar between groups (two sample t test, p = 0.77) and largely overlapping (in green CHT, in blue no CHT). The PCS mean metabolism (Figure  2 middle) was significantly (two sample t test, p = 0.02) lower in the CHT group, but there was a certain amount of subjects with similar values in the two groups. Using the PCS values, searching for the best cut-off to discriminate between CHT (lower than cut-off, under the orange line, Figure  2 left) and no CHT (higher than cut-off, above the orange line, Figure  2 left), we obtained an accuracy of 68%, with a great number of false attribution of patients in no CHT to the CHT group. Using the DAN values in searching for the best cut-off, we obtained a near chance result with only 50% of accuracy. On the contrary, putting together the two values of PCS and DAN with a PCS floating cut-off based on the DAN values (above and below the red line in Figure  2 left) we had an improvement in accuracy to 80%.

Figure 2
figure 2

The mean networks metabolism. On the left the prefrontal cerebellar system (PCS) and dorsal attention network (DAN) plotted in a scatter dot graph. In blue the no CHT subjects and in green the CHT subjects (arbitrary units). The red line was the best division between CHT and no CHT two groups using both PCS and DAN values. The orange line was the best division between CHT and no CHT using only PCS values. In the middle, the PCS values for the two groups (in green the CHT group = 1, in blue the no CHT group = 0). On the right the DAN, values for the two groups (in green the CHT group = 1, in blue the no CHT group = 0).

The metabolic values of the two networks in all patients were highly positively correlated (PCS-DAN r = 0.81, p < 0.001) and both exhibited a high inverse correlation with the age of subjects (AGE-DAN r = −0.51, p = 0.006; AGE-PCS r = −0.53, p = 0.004). No other clinical or demographic variable was correlated with the metabolic values extracted from the networks.

The ANCOVA models for CHT as independent and PCS mean metabolism as dependent variables were all significant controlling for different covariates: age (r2 = 0.50, CHT factor p = 0.01), depression (r2 = 0.36, CHT factor p = 0.02) or time elapsed from the end of treatment (r2 = 0.32, CHT factor p = 0.04).

Discussion

The two networks extracted in this work could be compared to those commonly extracted by many groups in the decomposition of fMRI/PET resting state signals, using blind data-driven multivariate techniques as principal component analysis, independent component analysis, subprofile scaling model, fuzzy clustering [35, 39, 40]. When covariation patterns of activity were extracted from brain resting state, the large-scale spatial networks, intrinsically contained within the mind architecture, were evidenced [20]. These techniques were used in the study of normal subjects to understand brain functioning and in pathological subjects to uncover characteristic pattern useful for diagnosis [4143].

Some of these networks [23] are more reliable than the others and are detectable in the resting state activity: default mode network, DAN (dorsal attentional network), sensorimotor network, visual networks, auditory network, left and right CEN (central executive networks), salience network (SN). These networks are also present in the activation study when elicited by specific tasks, the similarity of the active networks and resting brain-extracted networks is impressive [23].

Our implementation of MVPA with nu-SVM had the advantage of looking at groups differences considering at the same time the covariation patterns of the voxels in the brain. We found a hypometabolism in a network that we called PCS, composed by SN and to a lesser extent by CEN and OFC. A dysfunction of this network could potentially explain the neurocognitive, behavioural and mood alterations found in some chemotreated cancer survivors [13]. Some authors hypothesized that prefrontal dysfunction could be linked to acute depression and posttraumatic stress in those patients that later normalize [44]. However, our patients were not depressed or anxious and were cognitively spared. In our patients, CHT was effective, as demonstrated by the evaluation of total body PET, but nobody suffered severe physical effects. Also controlling for possible confounding effects as depression, time elapsed from CHT or age, the differences between groups in the network evidenced by MVPA was still significant. This result supported the role of CHT as the most probable responsible for the brain hypometabolism in our sample. We also found a spared metabolism in DAN that is important as a reference point for setting the PCS floating cut-off (Figure  2 left).

The meaning of these neural patterns could be inferred by meta-analytic analyses looking at the functional interpretations of brain networks in resting state [22].

In particular, the PCS included the cingulo-insular SN, an aspect of the attention and monitoring of the priority of stimuli processing and fronto-parietal CEN linked to working memory, planning, execution and action sequencing and the orbitofrontal areas related to reward, motivation and goal driven behaviour [22]. The DAN has constantly been associated with attention orientation, especially eyes orientation, and focusing into space, visuospatial integration, coordination and execution [22]. The presence in the DAN network of sensory-motor cortex and midbrain was extremely interesting and could reflect a link with coordination/execution, arousal and attentional processes.

All this functions are very flexible, involved in many tasks and are all often grouped under the name of frontal or executive functions; therefore, it is plausible to have seen them cluster together. The DAN performs less cognitive tasks, but highly interconnected with the aforementioned functions. In fact, focusing and orienting toward interesting things and planning/execution of goal-oriented actions are a top-down mechanism mirror of the bottom-up attention saliency: This was reflected in the DAN and PCS as very high positive correlation that we found in our sample. Both DAN and PCS are substrate of flexible, attention-based rapid processing skills that depend heavily on age and have a marked trend to decline with it [45]. In our sample, both DAN and PCS metabolism exhibited a high inverse correlation only with the age of subjects, and this is consistent with the functions that we attributed to these networks. We therefore could summarize this finding as a general inclination to have a global level of functioning that decreased in subjects with ageing. However, there is an important, although slight, difference in the two groups: The CHT seemed to dip only into the PCS, widely sparing the DAN metabolism. Age obviously represents a good coarse indicator of general frontal functioning, but different subjects of the same age could have different global frontal functioning due to other factors (genes, experiences, learning, epigenetic and environment). In this case, the best thing to study brain metabolism is to have a good indicator that could act as a set point as the DAN metabolism, unaffected by CHT and so equal to the pre-treatment level. This indicator definitively improved our capacity to discriminate between CHT and no CHT subjects (from 50% to 80%). We demonstrated a specific effect of the CHT (no other variable are significant in the comparison between groups, see Table  1 and ANCOVA analyses) on a specific network (PCS). CHT had a small effect, which is hard to detect or put in the right context with traditional univariate analyses [1013] or for behavioural comparison (no detectable differences between CHT and no CHT in any cognitive or psychological data).

Our findings could be juxtaposed with the recent work of Bruno and co-workers [46] that used the graph theoretical analysis to examine the connectome in chemotherapy-treated breast cancer survivors. They found a disrupted regional network in frontal, striatal and temporal areas, which is very similar to the PCS hypometabolic network.

Limitation

The limitation of the study was the presence in the sample of immunotherapy and the use of steroid at different doses or second-line treatments that do not allow us to distinguish if some chemotherapy drugs or combination could have more impact than the other. However, this limitation became less important, as we used a technique with increased sensitivity and we focused on a multivariate networks analysis prospective.

Future directions

We could hypothesize a future clinical use of the networks’ perspective in extracting PCS and DAN metabolic values from standard ROIs to identify patients at risk, using only one FDG-PET scan, and possibly to treat before clinical manifestation of cognitive fog or cancer fatigue occurs. This work should be replicated in greater numbers and with longitudinal design to demonstrate the clinical applicability, and hypometabolism should be put in relation to the development of clinical symptoms.

We could investigate possible mechanisms of neurotoxicity (e.g., proinflammatory cytokines, toxicity via brain blood barrier transporters and oxidative stress) via comparison of networks metabolism in groups stratified by genes' polymorphism (e.g., multi-drug resistant protein or MRP, interleukin or IL, tumour necrosis factor or TNF, apolipoprotein E or APOE, brain-derived neutrotrophic factor or BDNF).

Conclusion

The emerging concept consisted in the importance of the investigation of brain intrinsic networks, using multivariate statistical analyses, in cognitive chemotherapy, induced changes.

References

  1. Ahles TA, Saykin AJ: Candidate mechanisms for chemotherapy-induced cognitive changes. Nat Rev Cancer 2007, 7: 192–201. 10.1038/nrc2073

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Falleti MG, Sanfilippo A, Maruff P, Weih L, Phillips K-A: The nature and severity of cognitive impairment associated with adjuvant chemotherapy in women with breast cancer: a meta-analysis of the current literature. Brain Cogn 2005, 59: 60–70. 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.05.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Jansen CE, Miaskowski C, Dodd M, Dowling G, Kramer J: A metaanalysis of studies of the effects of cancer chemotherapy on various domains of cognitive function. Cancer 2005, 104: 2222–2233. 10.1002/cncr.21469

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Stemmer SM, Stears JC, Burton BS, Jones RB, Simon JH: White matter changes in patients with breast cancer treated with high-dose chemotherapy and autologous bone marrow support. AJNR 1994, 15: 1267–1273.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Brown MS, Stemmer SM, Simon JH, Stears JC, Jones RB, Cagnoni PJ, Sheeder JL: White matter disease induced by high-dose chemotherapy: longitudinal study with MR imaging and proton spectroscopy. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 1998, 19: 217–221.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. de Ruiter MB, Reneman L, Boogerd W, Veltman DJ, Caan M, Douaud G, Lavini C, Linn SC, Boven E, van Dam FS, Schagen SB: Late effects of high-dose adjuvant chemotherapy on white and gray matter in breast cancer survivors: Converging results from multimodal magnetic resonance imaging. Hum Brain Mapp 2011, 33: 2971–2983.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Inagaki M, Yoshikawa E, Matsuoka Y, Sugawara Y, Nakano T, Akechi T, Wada N, Imoto S, Murakami K, Uchitomi Y: Smaller regional volumes of brain gray and white matter demonstrated in breast cancer survivors exposed to adjuvant chemotherapy. Cancer 2007, 109: 146–156. 10.1002/cncr.22368

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Brown MS, Simon JH, Stemmer SM, Stears JC, Scherzinger A, Cagnoni PJ, Jones RB: MR and proton spectroscopy of white matter disease induced by high-dose chemotherapy with bone marrow transplant in advanced breast carcinoma. AJNR 1995, 16: 2013–2020.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Koppelmans V, de Ruiter MB, van der Lijn F, Boogerd W, Seynaeve C, van der Lugt A, Vrooman H, Niessen WJ, Breteler MM, Schagen SB: Global and focal brain volume in long-term breast cancer survivors exposed to adjuvant chemotherapy. Breast Cancer Res Treat 2012, 132: 1099–1106. 10.1007/s10549-011-1888-1

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Silverman DHS, Dy CJ, Castellon SA, Lai J, Pio BS, Abraham L, Waddell K, Petersen L, Phelps ME, Ganz PA: Altered frontocortical, cerebellar, and basal ganglia activity in adjuvant-treated breast cancer survivors 5–10 years after chemotherapy. Breast Cancer Res Treat 2007, 103: 303–311. 10.1007/s10549-006-9380-z

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Kesler SR, Bennett FC, Mahaffey ML, Spiegel D: Regional brain activation during verbal declarative memory in metastatic breast cancer. Clin Cancer Res 2009, 15: 6665–6673. 10.1158/1078-0432.CCR-09-1227

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. de Ruiter MB, Reneman L, Boogerd W, Veltman DJ, van Dam FSAM, Nederveen AJ, Boven E, Schagen SB: Cerebral hyporesponsiveness and cognitive impairment 10 years after chemotherapy for breast cancer. Hum Brain Mapp 2011, 32: 1206–1219. 10.1002/hbm.21102

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Ferguson RJ, McDonald BC, Saykin AJ, Ahles TA: Brain structure and function differences in monozygotic twins: possible effects of breast cancer chemotherapy. JCO 2007, 25: 3866–3870. 10.1200/JCO.2007.10.8639

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Collins B, Mackenzie J, Stewart A, Bielajew C, Verma S: Cognitive effects of chemotherapy in post-menopausal breast cancer patients 1 year after treatment. Psychooncology 2009, 18: 134–143. 10.1002/pon.1379

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Fan HGM, Houédé-Tchen N, Yi Q-L, Chemerynsky I, Downie FP, Sabate K, Tannock IF: Fatigue, menopausal symptoms, and cognitive function in women after adjuvant chemotherapy for breast cancer: 1- and 2-year follow-up of a prospective controlled study. JCO 2005, 23: 8025–8032. 10.1200/JCO.2005.01.6550

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Ahles TA, Saykin AJ, Furstenberg CT, Cole B, Mott LA, Skalla K, Whedon MB, Bivens S, Mitchell T, Greenberg ER, Silberfarb PM: Neuropsychologic impact of standard-dose systemic chemotherapy in long-term survivors of breast cancer and lymphoma. JCO 2002, 20: 485–493. 10.1200/JCO.20.2.485

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Castellon SA, Ganz PA, Bower JE, Petersen L, Abraham L, Greendale GA: Neurocognitive performance in breast cancer survivors exposed to adjuvant chemotherapy and tamoxifen. J Clin Exp Neuropsychology 2004, 26: 955–969. 10.1080/13803390490510905

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Baudino B, D'Agata F, Caroppo P, Castellano G, Cauda S, Manfredi M, Geda E, Castelli L, Mortara P, Orsi L, Cauda F, Sacco K, Ardito RB, Pinessi L, Geminiani G, Torta R, Bisi G: The chemotherapy long-term effect on cognitive functions and brain metabolism in lymphoma patients. Q J Nucl Med Mol Imaging 2012, 56: 559–568.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Wefel JS, Lenzi R, Theriault RL, Davis RN, Meyers CA: The cognitive sequelae of standard-dose adjuvant chemotherapy in women with breast carcinoma: results of a prospective, randomized, longitudinal trial. Cancer 2004, 100: 2292–2299. 10.1002/cncr.20272

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Biswal BB, Mennes M, Zuo X-N, Gohel S, Kelly C, Smith SM, Beckmann CF, Adelstein JS, Buckner RL, Colcombe S, Dogonowski AM, Ernst M, Fair D, Hampson M, Hoptman MJ, Hyde JS, Kiviniemi VJ, Kötter R, Li SJ, Lin CP, Lowe MJ, Mackay C, Madden DJ, Madsen KH, Margulies DS, Mayberg HS, McMahon K, Monk CS, Mostofsky SH: Toward discovery science of human brain function. PNAS 2010, 107: 4734–4739. 10.1073/pnas.0911855107

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Toro R, Fox PT, Paus T: Functional coactivation map of the human brain. Cereb Cortex 2008, 18: 2553–2559. 10.1093/cercor/bhn014

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Laird AR, Fox PM, Eickhoff SB, Turner JA, Ray KL, McKay DR, Glahn DC, Beckmann CF, Smith SM, Fox PT: Behavioral interpretations of intrinsic connectivity networks. J Cog Neurosci 2011, 23: 4022–4037. 10.1162/jocn_a_00077

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, Mackay CE, Filippini N, Watkins KE, Toro R, Laird AR, Beckmann CF: Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest. PNAS 2009, 106: 13040–13045. 10.1073/pnas.0905267106

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Friston KJ: Functional and effective connectivity in neuroimaging: a synthesis. Hum Brain Mapp 1994, 2: 56–78. 10.1002/hbm.460020107

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Habas C, Kamdar N, Nguyen D, Prater K, Beckmann CF, Menon V, Greicius MD: Distinct cerebellar contributions to intrinsic connectivity networks. J Neurosci 2009, 29: 8586–8594. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1868-09.2009

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Dosenbach NUF, Fair DA, Miezin FM, Cohen AL, Wenger KK, Dosenbach RA, Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE: Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. PNAS 2007, 104: 11073–11078. 10.1073/pnas.0704320104

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, Reiss AL, Greicius MD: Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci 2007, 27: 2349–2356. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME: Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. PNAS 2006, 103: 10046–10051. 10.1073/pnas.0604187103

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Guyon I, Weston J, Barnhill S, Vapnik V: Gene selection for cancer classification using support vector machines. Mach Learn 2002, 46: 389–422. 10.1023/A:1012487302797

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Alonso-Atienza F, Rojo-Álvarez JL, Rosado-Muñoz A, Vinagre JJ, García-Alberola A, Camps-Valls G: Feature selection using support vector machines and bootstrap methods for ventricular fibrillation detection. Expert Syst Appl 2012, 39: 1956–1967. 10.1016/j.eswa.2011.08.051

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. De Martino F, Valente G, Staeren N, Ashburner J, Goebel R, Formisano E: Combining multivariate voxel selection and support vector machines for mapping and classification of fMRI spatial patterns. Neuroimage 2008, 43: 44–58. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.06.037

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Pereira F, Mitchell T, Botvinick M: Machine learning classifiers and fMRI: a tutorial overview. Neuroimage 2009, 45: S199-S209. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Haxby JV, Gobbini MI, Furey ML, Ishai A, Schouten JL, Pietrini P: Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science 2001, 293: 2425–2430. 10.1126/science.1063736

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Haynes J-D, Rees G: Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans. Nat Rev Neurosci 2006, 7: 523–534. 10.1038/nrn1931

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Moeller JR, Strother SC: A regional covariance approach to the analysis of functional patterns in positron emission tomographic data. J Cereb Blood Flow Metabolism 1991, 11: A121-A135.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Kippenhan JS, Barker WW, Pascal S, Nagel J, Duara R: Evaluation of a neural-network classifier for PET scans of normal and Alzheimer's disease subjects. J Nucl Med 1992, 33: 1459–1467.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Vapnik VN: An overview of statistical learning theory. IEEE Trans Neural Netw 1999, 10: 988–999. 10.1109/72.788640

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Chang C, Lin C: LIBSVM: a library for support vector machines. ACM Trans Intell Syst Technol 2011,2(27–1):27–1.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Smolders A, De Martino F, Staeren N, Scheunders P, Sijbers J, Goebel R, Formisano E: Dissecting cognitive stages with time-resolved fMRI data: a comparison of fuzzy clustering and independent component analysis. Magn Reson Imaging 2007, 25: 860–868. 10.1016/j.mri.2007.02.018

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Formisano E, Esposito F, Di Salle F, Goebel R: Cortex-based independent component analysis of fMRI time series. Magn Reson Imaging 2004, 22: 1493–1504. 10.1016/j.mri.2004.10.020

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Habeck C, Foster NL, Perneczky R, Kurz A, Alexopoulos P, Koeppe RA, Drzezga A, Stern Y: Multivariate and univariate neuroimaging biomarkers of Alzheimer's disease. Neuroimage 2008, 40: 1503–1515. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.056

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Mingoia G, Wagner G, Langbein K, Maitra R, Smesny S, Dietzek M, Burmeister HP, Reichenbach JR, Schlösser RG, Gaser C, Sauer H, Nenadic I: Default mode network activity in schizophrenia studied at resting state using probabilistic ICA. Schizophr Res 2012, 138: 143–149. 10.1016/j.schres.2012.01.036

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Spetsieris PG, Ma Y, Dhawan V, Eidelberg D: Differential diagnosis of Parkinsonian syndromes using PCA-based functional imaging features. Neuroimage 2009, 45: 1241–1252. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.063

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Chiaravalloti A, Pagani M, Di Pietro B, Danieli R, Tavolozza M, Travascio L, Caracciolo CR, Simonetti G, Cantonetti M, Schillaci O: Is cerebral glucose metabolism affected by chemotherapy in patients with Hodgkin's lymphoma? Nucl Med Commun 2012, 34: 57–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Hedden T, Gabrieli JDE: Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 2004, 5: 87–96.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Bruno J, Hosseini SMH, Kesler S: Altered resting state functional brain network topology in chemotherapy-treated breast cancer survivors. Neurobiol Dis 2012, 48: 329–338. 10.1016/j.nbd.2012.07.009

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Authors would like to thank the patients who participated in the study and their families. We wish to thank Dr. Francesca Giunta and Dr. Chiara Caroppo for their contribution to this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Federico D'Agata.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

FD and BB have made substantial contributions to conception and design. FD, PC, BB, GG, LP, GC and GB are involved in the acquisition of data. FD, TC, BB, FC and PM are responsible for the analysis and interpretation of data. All authors have been involved in drafting the manuscript or revising it critically for the important intellectual content. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 2.0 International License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

D'Agata, F., Costa, T., Caroppo, P. et al. Multivariate analysis of brain metabolism reveals chemotherapy effects on prefrontal cerebellar system when related to dorsal attention network. EJNMMI Res 3, 22 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1186/2191-219X-3-22

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/2191-219X-3-22

Keywords